• You are currently viewing our forum as a guest, which gives you limited access to view most discussions and access our other features. By joining our free community, you will have access to additional post topics, communicate privately with other members (PM), view blogs, respond to polls, upload content, and access many other special features. Registration is fast, simple and absolutely free, so please join our community today! Just click here to register. You should turn your Ad Blocker off for this site or certain features may not work properly. If you have any problems with the registration process or your account login, please contact us by clicking here.

How the Brain Creates Personality: A New Theory

Vasilisa

Symbolic Herald
Joined
Feb 2, 2010
Messages
3,946
Instinctual Variant
so/sx
How the Brain Creates Personality: A New Theory
Are you a mover, a perceiver, a stimulator, or an adapter? Modes of thinking can be understood in terms of how the top and bottom—rather than right and left—parts of the brain interact.
STEPHEN M. KOSSLYN AND G. WAYNE MILLER
NOV 11 2013
The Atlantic

Excerpt:
It is possible to examine any object—including a brain—at different levels. Take the example of a building. If we want to know whether the house will have enough space for a family of five, we want to focus on the architectural level; if we want to know how easily it could catch fire, we want to focus on the materials level; and if we want to engineer a product for a brick manufacturer, we focus on molecular structure.

Similarly, if we want to know how the brain gives rise to thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, we want to focus on the bigger picture of how its structure allows it to store and process information—the architecture, as it were. To understand the brain at this level, we don’t have to know everything about the individual connections among brain cells or about any other biochemical process. We use a relatively high level of analysis, akin to architecture in buildings, to characterize relatively large parts of the brain.

People vary in the degree that they tend to rely on the top and bottom brain systems. This leads to four basic cognitive modes that underlie how a person approaches the world and interacts with other people.

To explain the Theory of Cognitive Modes, which specifies general ways of thinking that underlie how a person approaches the world and interacts with other people, we need to provide you with a lot of information. We want you to understand where this theory came from—that we didn’t just pull it out of a hat or make it up out of whole cloth. But there’s no need to lose the forest for the trees: there are only three key points that you will really need to keep in mind.

First, the top parts and the bottom parts of the brain have different functions. The top brain formulates and executes plans (which often involve deciding where to move objects or how to move the body in space), whereas the bottom brain classifies and interprets incoming information about the world. The two halves always work together; most important, the top brain uses information from the bottom brain to formulate its plans (and to reformulate them, as they unfold over time).

Second, according to the theory, people vary in the degree that they tend to rely on each of the two brain systems for functions that are optional (i.e., not dictated by the immediate situation): Some people tend to rely heavily on both brain systems, some rely heavily on the bottom brain system but not the top, some rely heavily on the top but not the bottom, and some don’t rely heavily on either system.

Third, these four scenarios define four basic cognitive modes— general ways of thinking that underlie how a person approaches the world and interacts with other people. According to the Theory of Cognitive Modes, each of us has a particular dominant cognitive mode, which affects how we respond to situations we encounter and how we relate to others. The possible modes are: Mover Mode, Perceiver Mode, Stimulator Mode, and Adaptor Mode.

Systems, Not Dichotomies
We use what researchers have learned to present a new theory of brain function that hinges on how the top and bottom parts of the brain interact. But we do not try to characterize the top and bottom parts of the brain in terms of a simple dichotomy or set of dichotomies, which was exactly what was done with the existing and well-known division of the brain into two halves: namely the left versus the right, the dominant pop-culture brain story of the last few decades. You have probably heard of this theory, in which the left and right halves of the brain are characterized, respectively, as logical versus intuitive, verbal versus perceptual, analytic versus synthetic, and so forth. The trouble is that none of these sweeping generalizations has stood up to careful scientific scrutiny. The differences between the left and right sides of the brain are nuanced, and simple, sweeping dichotomies do not in fact explain how the two sides function.

When considering large portions of the brain, we need to think about systems—not dichotomies. A system has inputs and outputs, and a set of constituent components that work together to produce appropriate outputs for particular inputs.

A bicycle is a familiar system: The inputs are forces that push down on the pedals, slight movements of the rider’s body made in the act of balancing, and force that moves the handlebars. The components include the seat, the wheels, the handlebars, the pedals, the gears, the chain, and so forth. The outputs are the bike’s forward motion, keeping upright, and going in a specific direction, all at the same time. Crucially, the components are designed to work together to produce appropriate outputs for the system as a whole—for the entire bike.

The same is true of the brain: It has different areas that do different things, and the result of the brain areas’ working together is to produce appropriate outputs (such as your avoiding an object) for particular inputs (such as specific sights and sounds). For instance, if you see a car roaring toward you, you jump out of the way.

Top Brain, Bottom Brain
The Theory of Cognitive Modes is based on organizing the brain into two major parts, top and bottom—each of which we will characterize as a large system that processes information in particular ways. As we show, we gain a lot by organizing the brain into these two large systems, noting how constituent parts work together. Let’s begin by being clear about what we mean by the top and bottom parts: Look at the diagram of a side view of the brain, which shows the cerebral cortex, the thin outer covering of the brain where most of the bodies of neurons reside. The cerebral cortex is where most cognitive activities arise—and we focus almost entirely on the cerebral cortex (not the “inner brain” structures that are located under the cortex, in the interior of the brain, and are involved in emotion and many automatic functions such as controlling arousal and hunger).

3c5bec656.jpg

The lobes of the brain. Note that the crease along the top of the temporal lobe is the Sylvian fissure, which divides most of the bottom brain from the top brain. (Simon and Schuster)
The diagram notes the locations of the four lobes of the brain— occipital, temporal, frontal, and parietal—and the location of the Sylvian fissure, a large, highly visible crease that roughly divides the brain into top and bottom parts. Each of the lobes implements many relatively specialized systems, but for our purposes it will be most useful to group the lobes into two large processing systems: The occipital and temporal lobes are in the bottom part of the brain, and the parietal and most of the frontal lobes are in the top part of the brain. A further neuroanatomical distinction must also be made: The frontal lobe itself can be divided into a top and bottom portion, based on how these portions are connected to the parietal and temporal lobes, respectively. Thus the brain neatly divides into a top and bottom part.

The top and bottom portions of the brain have very different functions. This fact was first discovered in the context of visual perception, and it was supported in 1982 in a landmark report by National Medal of Science winner Mortimer Mishkin and Leslie G. Ungerleider, of the National Institute of Mental Health. This trailblazing study, which went largely unnoticed in the popular culture, examined rhesus monkeys. Their brains process visual information in much the same way as the human brain.

The scientists trained monkeys to perform two tasks. In the first task, the monkeys had to learn to recognize which of two shapes concealed a bit of food. The shapes were three-dimensional objects (such as a striped prismatic block) that concealed small cups, one of which contained a tasty morsel. The objects were shuffled randomly each time they were presented, but the same object covered the food every time, so the animals needed to learn to recognize it in order to find the food. In the second task, both objects were identical gray placards; both placards concealed small cups, one of which contained food. Now, a small cylindrical block was placed closer to whichever placard concealed the food. The location of the cylinder was shuffled randomly each time the choice was presented, so that it was closer to one of the placards than the other—but the food was always under the placard that was closest to the cylinder. The monkeys needed to learn to recognize which placard was closest to the cylinder in order to find the food.

In short, one task required learning to recognize shape, whereas the other required learning to recognize relative location.

After each animal had mastered the two tasks, a part of its brain was surgically removed. Some animals had a portion of the bottom brain taken out (the lower part of the temporal lobe), whereas others had a portion of the top brain taken out (the rear part of the parietal lobe). The results of these operations were dramatic: The animals that had a portion of the bottom brain removed no longer could do the shape task—and could not be taught to perform it again—but they could still perform the location task well. The animals that had a portion of the top brain removed had exactly the opposite problem: They could no longer do the location task, and could not relearn how to perform it—but they could still do the shape task well.

The top and bottom brain play specialized roles in memory, attention, decision making, planning, and emotion.
Many later studies, including those that relied on using neuroimaging to monitor activity in the human brain while people performed tasks analogous to the ones the monkeys had performed, have led to the same conclusion: Processing in the temporal lobe (located in the bottom brain) plays a crucial role in visual recognition—the sense that we’ve seen an object before, that it’s familiar (I’ve seen that cat before)—whereas processing in the parietal lobe (in the top brain) plays a crucial role in allowing us to register spatial relations (One object is to the left side of the other).

These functions occur relatively close to where neural connections deliver inputs from the eyes and ears—but processing doesn’t just stop there. Rather, information about what an object is and where it is located flows to other brain areas, which do different things with that information. Researchers have shown that the top and bottom brain play specialized roles in functions as diverse as memory, attention, decision making, planning, and emotion.

The bottom part of the brain is largely concerned with processing inputs from the senses and using them to activate the appropriate memories about relevant objects and events. For example, when you see a friend’s face in a sea of strangers, you recognize her face because the input from the eyes acts like a key that unlocks the memory of your friend. Once you’ve activated the relevant memories, you know things about the stimulus that are not apparent in what you see—such as that she likes cappuccino, has had a lot of experience working in your industry, and often gives good advice.

Knowing what you are seeing or hearing is sometimes an end in itself (such as when watching TV), but not often. Usually, we want to know what’s going on around us so that we can specify goals and figure out how to achieve them. For instance, you might decide to ask your friend to get together at a favorite coffeehouse to have a cup of cappuccino—and plan to ask her advice about a problem you are having at work.

Where do such plans come from, and how are they acted on?

Devising and carrying out plans is the realm of the top-brain system. In particular, the top parts of the frontal lobe are concerned with these functions. But how does the top brain know what is being perceived? Information about where objects are located in space is so important for making plans that it is processed directly in the top brain; we need to know where objects are located in order to decide how to move them or how to move our bodies as we seek to approach or avoid them. (In our example, without such information, you couldn’t have known how to thread your way through the crowd to reach and talk to your friend.) But we need to know more than just where objects are located—we also need to know what they are. Such information from the bottom brain goes to the top brain, allowing the top brain to use information about the nature of objects being perceived.

The bottom-brain system organizes signals from the senses. The top-brain system uses information about the environment to figure out which goals to try to achieve.
The top part of the frontal lobe also contains numerous areas that control movements. Because our movements occur in our immediate environment, to program them appropriately our brains need to know where objects are located—to reach for them, step over them, run from them, and so forth. To walk over to your friend, you need to know where she is relative to your body; to talk to her, you need to know where she is facing, and you need to position yourself close enough (but not too close!) so that she can hear you easily.

The top parts of our frontal lobe can take into account the confluence of information about “what’s out there,” our emotional reactions to it, and our goals. They then play a crucial role in allowing us to formulate plans, make decisions, and direct attention in particular ways (in part by connections to the parietal lobes); they allow us to figure out what to do, given our goals and our emotional reactions to the unfolding events that surround us.

The bottom-brain system organizes signals from the senses, simultaneously comparing what is being perceived with all the information previously stored in memory—and then uses the results of such comparisons to classify and interpret the object or event that gives rise to the input signals.

The top-brain system uses information about the surrounding environment (in combination with other sorts of information, such as emotional reactions and need for food or drink) to figure out which goals to try to achieve. It actively formulates plans, generates expectations about what should happen when a plan is executed, and then—as the plan is being carried out—compares what is happening with what was expected, adjusting the plan accordingly (for example, by adjusting your grip as the phone starts to slip from your hand).

Four Cognitive Modes
Four distinct cognitive modes emerge from how the top-brain and bottom-brain systems can interact. The degree to which each of the brain systems is used spans a continuum, ranging from highly utilized to minimally utilized. Nevertheless, for our purposes it is useful to divide the continuum into “high” and “low” categories.


Mover Mode results when the top- and bottom-brain systems are both highly utilized. When people think in this mode, they are inclined to make and act on plans (using the top-brain system) and to register the consequences of doing so (using the bottom-brain system), subsequently adjusting plans on the basis of feedback. According to our theory, people who habitually rely on Mover Mode typically are most comfortable in positions that allow them to plan, act, and see the consequences of their actions.

Perceiver Mode results when the bottom-brain system is highly utilized but the top-brain system is not. When people think in this mode, they use the bottom-brain system to try to make sense of what they perceive in depth; they interpret what they experience, put it in context, and try to understand the implications. However, by definition, people who are operating in Perceiver Mode do not often initiate detailed or complex plans.

Stimulator Mode results when the top-brain system is highly utilized but the bottom-brain system is not. According to our theory, when people rely on Stimulator Mode they may be creative and original, but they do not always know when “enough is enough”—their actions can be disruptive, and they may not adjust their behavior appropriately.

Adaptor Mode results when neither the top- nor the bottom-brain system is highly utilized. People who are thinking in this mode are not caught up in initiating plans, nor are they fully focused on classifying and interpreting what they experience. Instead, our theory predicts that they are open to becoming absorbed by local events and immediate imperatives. They should tend to be action-oriented, and responsive to ongoing situations.
< Continue Reading >
 

reckful

New member
Joined
Jul 6, 2013
Messages
656
MBTI Type
INTJ
Enneagram
5
Maybe I'm missing something, but here's my quick take:

The authors start out by noting that much of the pop-psych stuff you read about left/right brain has failed to stand up to "scientific scrutiny." So far, so good, as far as I know.

Then they explain that, based on respectable studies, it appears that various mental processes can fairly be said to be "top brain" or "bottom brain" things. Again, so far, so good, as far as I know.

But then... they introduce this "theory" they have that "predicts" that people will tend to be one of four types (Movers, Perceivers, Stimulators and Adaptors) based on whether they "tend to rely heavily on both brain systems" or "rely heavily on the bottom brain system but not the top" or "rely heavily on the top but not the bottom" or "don’t rely heavily on either system."

But here's the thing: They don't cite a single study in support of their "theory." The only studies they describe — both in the Atlantic article you've linked to and this Wall Street Journal article — are studies that support the idea that the top and bottom brains perform different functions, not studies that suggest that someone will have a somewhat hardwired tendency to be a top-brain person or a bottom-brain person or a both-halves person or a neither-half person.

And yet, as I said, they open their article by criticizing left/right brain theories on the grounds that they've failed to withstand "scientific scrutiny." And I'd also say that both articles are written in a way that would lead a casual reader to think that the top brain and bottom brain studies they do describe somehow support their "four types" theory.

In any case, if there's no real evidence yet for their theory, it's arguably a little early to be publishing a book aimed at the general public and publishing articles in mass-market periodicals and saying things like (see the WSJ article) "Oprah Winfrey is X type" and "Elizabeth Taylor was Y type" and you can take our little test and find out what type you are!

And in case you're wondering if Kosslyn has any plans to test his theory himself... if you look at the Preface (at Google Books), it appears not. The authors tell us they "hope that this book will inspire a new round of studies" — by somebody else, I guess.

I also think it's kind of odd that the authors make no mention (in either of those two articles) of the Big Five factors and how they think their "types" relate to those. Perhaps that's in their book somewhere.

I suppose it's possible the real "theory" behind the book is: "If that goofball Gladwell can make big bucks writing pop-psych books, why not us?" :alttongue:
 
Last edited:
B

brainheart

Guest
I'm inclined to agree with [MENTION=18736]reckful[/MENTION] on this one. I did take the test, though. It said I was primarily a perceiver and sometimes an adaptor which essentially fits, I guess. That sort of knowledge doesn't feel particularly useful to me, though. It's kind of a, well, duh on the perceiver end, hmm maybe but not really on the adaptor side of things.

More importantly, does anyone else look at a picture of a brain and imagine that it would make a squeaky sound if you were to maw it with your gums, kind of like rubbing a dolphin's head?
 

freeeekyyy

Cheeseburgers
Joined
Feb 13, 2010
Messages
1,384
MBTI Type
INTJ
Enneagram
5w4
Instinctual Variant
sx/sp
I'm apparently a mover, so make decisions, but consider context. I guess that's useful information, except that it's self apparent and already known. It reminds me a lot of MBTI, when it's limited to just the dichotomies.
 
Top